Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 5 de 5
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38149680

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Pictures drawn by people with aphasia (PWA) are often more challenging to understand than those drawn by healthy people. There are two types of objects: those that tend to be drawn symbolically (symbolically drawn objects-SOs) and those that are likely to be drawn realistically (realistically drawn objects-ROs). AIMS: To compare the identification rate and number of misunderstanding types between SOs and ROs drawn by PWA and healthy controls (HCs). To reveal trends in the misunderstandings of drawings by PWA, and to identify the language or cognitive abilities related to the identification rate of pictures drawn by PWA. METHODS & PROCEDURES: We designed a drawing task involving SOs and ROs. A total of 18 PWA and 30 HCs completed the task, and respondents identified the drawings. The identification rate and number of misunderstandings were analysed with two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) including group (PWA and HCs) and object type (SOs and ROs). The misunderstandings were divided into four categories varying in semantic and morphological similarity; these ratios were examined with a chi-square test. The relationships of language and cognitive abilities with the identification rate were investigated with multiple regression analyses. OUTCOMES & RESULTS: There was a significant effect of the interaction between group and object type on the identification rate (F(1.1387) = 3.90, Mean Squared Error (MSE) = 4139.67, p = 0.04): the identification rates for ROs were lower in the PWA than in the HCs. For the number of misunderstanding types, an interaction was observed between group and object type (F(1.56) = 8.26, MSE = 26.93, p < 0.01): the number of misunderstanding types for ROs in the PWA was greater than that in the HCs. The misunderstanding patterns differed between ROs and SOs (χ2 (3) = 694.30, p < 0.001, V = 0.37). ROs were semantically related, whereas SOs were morphologically related. The identification rates of ROs and SOs were correlated only with Kanji writing scores (ROs: ß = 3.66, p = 0.01; SOs: ß = 6.57, p < 0.01). CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS: In drawings by the PWA, SOs had a higher identification rate, while ROs had a lower identification rate and a greater variety of misunderstandings. SOs may increase drawing motivation. Interventions to improve the identifiability of SOs and ROs should reflect each character. Identification rates were correlated only with Kanji writing scores. The PWA, whose native language was Japanese and had preserved Kanji writing abilities, and their communication abilities may be increased through drawing. WHAT THIS PAPER ADDS: What is already known on the subject PWA often have impaired drawing abilities and draw pictures that third parties misinterpret. Some objects tend to be drawn symbolically, and some are drawn realistically. However, it is not clear whether there is a difference between these types of drawings depicted by PWA in identifiability and the tendency to be misunderstood by ordinary people. In addition, the relationships between language or cognitive abilities and the identification rate of drawn pictures are not clear. What this paper adds to the existing knowledge The identification rate differed between SOs and ROs. In drawings by PWA, SOs had a higher identification rate, while ROs had a lower identification rate and the greatest variety of misunderstandings. Approximately half of the misunderstandings were related to the target object. SOs tended to be confused with morphologically related objects, while ROs tended to be confused with semantically related objects. Identification rates were correlated only with Kanji writing scores. What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work? To motivate PWA's drawing, it is suitable to begin with SOs. Examining drawing ability from the perspective of SOs and ROs increases the chance of identifying drawing ability. PWA whose native language is Japanese and have preserved Kanji writing abilities may be able to increase their communication abilities through drawing.

2.
Eur Neurol ; 83(4): 395-403, 2020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32784305

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Topographical disorientation is one of the early symptoms of Alzheimer's disease (AD). The nature of this symptom, however, remains unclear. OBJECTIVE: The aim of the study was to investigate egocentric and allocentric spatial cognition in patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) and early AD. PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS: The participants consisted of normal healthy volunteers (n = 23), patients with aMCI (n = 26), and patients with early AD (n = 22). We administered the card placing test (CPT), in which a subject was required to recreate an array of 3 cards, each of which was randomly placed on 8 grids around the individual, before (part A) and after (part B) the individual's rotation. With this design, the CPT can reveal an individual's ability to represent spatial information either egocentrically (CPT-A) or allocentrically (CPT-B). A qualitative analysis of errors in performing the CPT was also conducted. RESULTS: Compared with the controls, the aMCI patients showed significantly poorer CPT-B performance, while there was no significant difference in CPT-A performance between these 2 groups. In contrast, the AD patients demonstrated significantly poorer performance on both the CPT-A and CPT-B than the controls and aMCI patients. There was no significant difference in the profile of errors on the CPT-B between the controls and aMCI patients, whereas there was a notable difference in those on the CPT-A between the controls and AD patients and the aMCI and AD patients. CONCLUSION: Allocentric spatial cognition is selectively impaired in aMCI patients, while an egocentric spatial cognition is additionally impaired in AD patients.


Assuntos
Doença de Alzheimer/complicações , Disfunção Cognitiva/complicações , Confusão/etiologia , Navegação Espacial , Idoso , Idoso de 80 Anos ou mais , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Testes Neuropsicológicos
3.
Case Rep Neurol Med ; 2019: 2929782, 2019.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31061739

RESUMO

We presented a case of a 19-year-old woman who suffered from anti-N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor encephalitis associated with ovarian teratoma. The patient showed a variety of higher visual symptoms which changed over the recovery phase of the disease. In chronological order, she experienced cortical blindness, amblyopia, dyschromatopsia, static form agnosia, and prosopagnosia. Among these symptoms, the most intriguing was the static form agnosia. Although she could recognize the forms of moving objects, she could not make out those of stationary ones. All of these visual symptoms were transient, implying that she might have incidentally regained the function of the distinct cortical visual areas in the time course of follow-up. This case further suggests that visual functions concerned with the perceptions of static form and form-from-motion could be dissociable and may rely on distinct brain regions.

4.
Rinsho Shinkeigaku ; 56(12): 837-845, 2016 12 28.
Artigo em Japonês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27890879

RESUMO

We recently developed a new clinical test named card placing test (CPT) which can assess a subject's ability to deal with visuospatial information. The CPT requires a subject to recreate an array of three cards, each of which was randomly placed on eight grids around the subject, before (CPT-A) and after the subject's rotation (CPT-B). With this design, the CPT can assess a subject's ability to represent visuospatial information either egocentrically (CPT-A) or allocentrically (CPT-B). We administered the test on two patients with topographical disorientation; one with egocentric disorientation and another with heading disorientation. The patient with egocentric disorientation demonstrated poor performances on both CPT-A and CPT-B. The patient with heading disorientation, on the other hand, showed good record results for CPT-A but poor ones for CPT-B. An implication is that the patient with egocentric disorientation had disorder in an egocentric reference frame per se, while the patient with heading disorientation could not integrate information on the spatial locations derived from an egocentric reference frame with that on changes of the body directions. We suggest that the CPT is a simple and useful clinical test to evaluate patients with topographical disorientation.


Assuntos
Confusão/diagnóstico , Confusão/psicologia , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Orientação , Percepção Espacial , Percepção Visual , Idoso , Córtex Cerebral/fisiopatologia , Confusão/fisiopatologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Lobo Parietal/fisiopatologia
5.
Case Rep Neurol Med ; 2015: 396802, 2015.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26526327

RESUMO

We report the case of heading disorientation following ischemic stroke involving the right posteromedial areas. The patient was administered a new test named the Card Placing Test during which a subject was required to recreate an array of three cards, each of which was randomly placed on eight grids around the subject, before and after the subject's rotation. Qualitative analysis of his performance after rotation revealed that over half of the errors comprised transposition and rotational offset. His score on the Card Placing Test was compared with those of normal controls (n = 11). The results showed that his score on Card Placing Test after rotation was significantly lower than those of controls, whereas there was no significant difference between the case and controls in profile of error types. We infer that the heading disorientation observed in the present case was a result of a derangement of a short-term buffer that integrated information on spatial locations of objects with changes in body directions.

SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...